uni-leipzig-open-access/json/s13414-023-02763-9

1 line
34 KiB
Plaintext

{"status":"ok","message-type":"work","message-version":"1.0.0","message":{"indexed":{"date-parts":[[2023,10,26]],"date-time":"2023-10-26T12:18:56Z","timestamp":1698322736220},"reference-count":67,"publisher":"Springer Science and Business Media LLC","issue":"8","license":[{"start":{"date-parts":[[2023,8,2]],"date-time":"2023-08-02T00:00:00Z","timestamp":1690934400000},"content-version":"tdm","delay-in-days":0,"URL":"https:\/\/creativecommons.org\/licenses\/by\/4.0"},{"start":{"date-parts":[[2023,8,2]],"date-time":"2023-08-02T00:00:00Z","timestamp":1690934400000},"content-version":"vor","delay-in-days":0,"URL":"https:\/\/creativecommons.org\/licenses\/by\/4.0"}],"funder":[{"DOI":"10.13039\/501100002347","name":"Bundesministerium f\u00fcr Bildung und Forschung","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","award":["M526300"]},{"DOI":"10.13039\/501100008678","name":"Universit\u00e4t Leipzig","doi-asserted-by":"crossref"}],"content-domain":{"domain":["link.springer.com"],"crossmark-restriction":false},"short-container-title":["Atten Percept Psychophys"],"published-print":{"date-parts":[[2023,11]]},"abstract":"<jats:title>Abstract<\/jats:title><jats:p>The human auditory system is believed to represent regularities inherent in auditory information in internal models. Sounds not matching the standard regularity (deviants) elicit prediction error, alerting the system to information not explainable within currently active models. Here, we examine the widely neglected characteristic of deviants bearing predictive information themselves. In a modified version of the oddball paradigm, using higher-order regularities, we set up different expectations regarding the sound following a deviant. Higher-order regularities were defined by the relation of pitch within tone pairs (rather than absolute pitch of individual tones). In a deviant detection task participants listened to oddball sequences including two deviant types following diametrically opposed rules: one occurred mostly in succession (high repetition probability) and the other mostly in isolation (low repetition probability). Participants in Experiment 1 were not informed (na\u00efve), whereas in Experiment 2 they were made aware of the repetition rules. Response times significantly decreased from first to second deviant when repetition probability was high\u2014albeit more in the presence of explicit rule knowledge. There was no evidence of a facilitation effect when repetition probability was low. Significantly more false alarms occurred in response to standards following high compared with low repetition probability deviants, but only in participants aware of the repetition rules. These findings provide evidence that not only deviants violating lower- but also higher-order regularities can inform predictions about auditory events. More generally, they confirm the utility of this new paradigm to gather further insights into the predictive properties of the human brain.<\/jats:p>","DOI":"10.3758\/s13414-023-02763-9","type":"journal-article","created":{"date-parts":[[2023,8,2]],"date-time":"2023-08-02T22:01:19Z","timestamp":1691013679000},"page":"2731-2750","update-policy":"http:\/\/dx.doi.org\/10.1007\/springer_crossmark_policy","source":"Crossref","is-referenced-by-count":0,"title":["Deviants violating higher-order auditory regularities can become predictive and facilitate behaviour"],"prefix":"10.3758","volume":"85","author":[{"ORCID":"http:\/\/orcid.org\/0000-0003-4037-8502","authenticated-orcid":false,"given":"Nina","family":"Coy","sequence":"first","affiliation":[]},{"given":"Alexandra","family":"Bendixen","sequence":"additional","affiliation":[]},{"given":"Sabine","family":"Grimm","sequence":"additional","affiliation":[]},{"given":"Urte","family":"Roeber","sequence":"additional","affiliation":[]},{"given":"Erich","family":"Schr\u00f6ger","sequence":"additional","affiliation":[]}],"member":"297","published-online":{"date-parts":[[2023,8,2]]},"reference":[{"issue":"12","key":"2763_CR1","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"1850","DOI":"10.1111\/j.1530-0277.2000.tb01989.x","volume":"24","author":"J Ahveninen","year":"2000","unstructured":"Ahveninen, J., J\u00e4\u00e4skel\u00e4inen, I. P., Pekkonen, E., Hallberg, A., Hietanen, M., N\u00e4\u00e4t\u00e4nen, R., Schr\u00f6ger, E., & Sillanaukee, P. (2000). Increased distractibility by task-irrelevant sound changes in abstinent alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24(12), 1850\u20131854. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1111\/j.1530-0277.2000.tb01989.x","journal-title":"Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research"},{"key":"2763_CR2","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","unstructured":"Bates, D., M\u00e4chler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1). https:\/\/doi.org\/10.18637\/jss.v067.i01","DOI":"10.18637\/jss.v067.i01"},{"issue":"8 MAR","key":"2763_CR3","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"1","DOI":"10.3389\/fnins.2014.00060","volume":"8","author":"A Bendixen","year":"2014","unstructured":"Bendixen, A. (2014). Predictability effects in auditory scene analysis: A review. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8(8 MAR), 1\u201316. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.3389\/fnins.2014.00060","journal-title":"Frontiers in Neuroscience"},{"issue":"5","key":"2763_CR4","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"928","DOI":"10.1016\/j.clinph.2012.11.012","volume":"124","author":"A Bendixen","year":"2013","unstructured":"Bendixen, A., & Andersen, S. K. (2013). Measuring target detection performance in paradigms with high event rates. Clinical Neurophysiology, 124(5), 928\u2013940. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/j.clinph.2012.11.012","journal-title":"Clinical Neurophysiology"},{"key":"2763_CR5","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","unstructured":"Benjamin, D. J., Berger, J. O., Johannesson, M., Nosek, B. A., Wagenmakers, E.-J., Berk, R., Bollen, K. A., Brembs, B., Brown, L., Camerer, C., Cesarini, D., Chambers, C. D., Clyde, M., Cook, T. D., De Boeck, P., Dienes, Z., Dreber, A., Easwaran, K., Efferson, C., \u2026, Johnson, V. E. (2018). Redefine statistical significance. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(1), 6\u201310. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1038\/s41562-017-0189-z","DOI":"10.1038\/s41562-017-0189-z"},{"issue":"4","key":"2763_CR6","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"608","DOI":"10.1111\/j.1469-8986.2008.00660.x","volume":"45","author":"S Berti","year":"2008","unstructured":"Berti, S. (2008). Cognitive control after distraction: Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) dissociate between different processes of attentional allocation. Psychophysiology, 45(4), 608\u2013620. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1111\/j.1469-8986.2008.00660.x","journal-title":"Psychophysiology"},{"issue":"1","key":"2763_CR7","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"395","DOI":"10.32614\/rj-2018-017","volume":"10","author":"PC B\u00fcrkner","year":"2018","unstructured":"B\u00fcrkner, P. C. (2018). Advanced Bayesian multilevel modeling with the R package brms. Royal Journal, 10(1), 395\u2013411. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.32614\/rj-2018-017","journal-title":"Royal Journal"},{"key":"2763_CR8","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","unstructured":"B\u00fcrkner, P.-C. (2017). brms: An R Package for Bayesian Multilevel Models Using Stan. Journal of Statistical Software, 80(1), 1\u201328. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.18637\/jss.v080.i01","DOI":"10.18637\/jss.v080.i01"},{"issue":"3","key":"2763_CR9","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"301","DOI":"10.1097\/00001756-200502280-00020","volume":"16","author":"V Carral","year":"2005","unstructured":"Carral, V., Corral, M. J., & Escera, C. (2005). Auditory event-related potentials as a function of abstract change magnitude. NeuroReport, 16(3), 301\u2013305. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1097\/00001756-200502280-00020","journal-title":"NeuroReport"},{"issue":"3","key":"2763_CR10","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"181","DOI":"10.1017\/S0140525X12000477","volume":"36","author":"A Clark","year":"2013","unstructured":"Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181\u2013204. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1017\/S0140525X12000477","journal-title":"Behavioral and Brain Sciences"},{"issue":"3\/4","key":"2763_CR11","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"169","DOI":"10.1080\/25742442.2022.2094657","volume":"5","author":"N Coy","year":"2022","unstructured":"Coy, N., Bendixen, A., Grimm, S., Roeber, U., & Schr\u00f6ger, E. (2022). Is the oddball just an odd-one-out? The predictive value of rule-violating events. Auditory Perception & Cognition, 5(3\/4), 169\u2013191. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1080\/25742442.2022.2094657","journal-title":"Auditory Perception & Cognition"},{"issue":"1","key":"2763_CR12","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"1","DOI":"10.3758\/s13428-014-0458-y","volume":"47","author":"JR de Leeuw","year":"2015","unstructured":"de Leeuw, J. R. (2015). jsPsych: A JavaScript library for creating behavioral experiments in a web browser. Behavior Research Methods, 47(1), 1\u201312. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.3758\/s13428-014-0458-y","journal-title":"Behavior Research Methods"},{"issue":"3","key":"2763_CR13","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"167","DOI":"10.1016\/S0304-3940(00)01175-7","volume":"287","author":"D Deacon","year":"2000","unstructured":"Deacon, D., Gomes, H., Nousak, J. M., Ritter, W., & Javitt, D. (2000). Effect of frequency separation and stimulus rate on the mismatch negativity: An examination of the issue of refractoriness in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 287(3), 167\u2013170. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/S0304-3940(00)01175-7","journal-title":"Neuroscience Letters"},{"issue":"June","key":"2763_CR14","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"1","DOI":"10.3389\/fpsyt.2020.00468","volume":"11","author":"K Fitzgerald","year":"2020","unstructured":"Fitzgerald, K., & Todd, J. (2020). Making sense of mismatch negativity. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11(June), 1\u201319. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.3389\/fpsyt.2020.00468","journal-title":"Frontiers in Psychiatry"},{"issue":"1456","key":"2763_CR15","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"815","DOI":"10.1098\/rstb.2005.1622","volume":"360","author":"K Friston","year":"2005","unstructured":"Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences, 360(1456), 815\u2013836. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1098\/rstb.2005.1622","journal-title":"Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences"},{"issue":"2","key":"2763_CR16","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"127","DOI":"10.1038\/nrn2787","volume":"11","author":"K Friston","year":"2010","unstructured":"Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(2), 127\u2013138. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1038\/nrn2787","journal-title":"Nature Reviews Neuroscience"},{"issue":"3","key":"2763_CR17","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"453","DOI":"10.1016\/j.clinph.2008.11.029","volume":"120","author":"MI Garrido","year":"2009","unstructured":"Garrido, M. I., Kilner, J. M., Stephan, K. E., & Friston, K. (2009). The mismatch negativity: A review of underlying mechanisms. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120(3), 453\u2013463. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/j.clinph.2008.11.029","journal-title":"Clinical Neurophysiology"},{"issue":"Pt. 2","key":"2763_CR18","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"257","DOI":"10.1348\/000711005X65753","volume":"59","author":"MJ Hautus","year":"2006","unstructured":"Hautus, M. J., & Lee, A. (2006). Estimating sensitivity and bias in a yes\/no task. The British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 59(Pt. 2), 257\u2013273. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1348\/000711005X65753","journal-title":"The British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology"},{"key":"2763_CR19","unstructured":"Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian journal of statistics, 65\u201370."},{"issue":"7","key":"2763_CR20","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"2139","DOI":"10.3758\/s13414-019-01779-4","volume":"81","author":"B Hommel","year":"2019","unstructured":"Hommel, B. (2019). Theory of Event Coding (TEC) V2.0: Representing and controlling perception and action. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81(7), 2139\u20132154. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.3758\/s13414-019-01779-4","journal-title":"Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics"},{"issue":"5","key":"2763_CR21","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"849","DOI":"10.1017\/S0140525X01000103","volume":"24","author":"B Hommel","year":"2001","unstructured":"Hommel, B., M\u00fcsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 849\u2013878. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1017\/S0140525X01000103","journal-title":"Behavioral and Brain Sciences"},{"issue":"4","key":"2763_CR22","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"434","DOI":"10.1016\/j.jml.2007.11.007","volume":"59","author":"TF Jaeger","year":"2008","unstructured":"Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 434\u2013446. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/j.jml.2007.11.007","journal-title":"Journal of Memory and Language"},{"key":"2763_CR23","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","unstructured":"Kanai, R., Komura, Y., Shipp, S., & Friston, K. (2015). Cerebral hierarchies: predictive processing, precision and the pulvinar. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370(1668), 20140169. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1098\/rstb.2014.0169","DOI":"10.1098\/rstb.2014.0169"},{"issue":"4","key":"2763_CR24","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"905","DOI":"10.3758\/s13428-013-0330-5","volume":"45","author":"KN Kirby","year":"2013","unstructured":"Kirby, K. N., & Gerlanc, D. (2013). BootES: An R package for bootstrap confidence intervals on effect sizes. Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 905\u2013927. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.3758\/s13428-013-0330-5","journal-title":"Behavior Research Methods"},{"issue":"12","key":"2763_CR25","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"712","DOI":"10.1016\/j.tins.2004.10.007","volume":"27","author":"DC Knill","year":"2004","unstructured":"Knill, D. C., & Pouget, A. (2004). The Bayesian brain: The role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation. Trends in Neurosciences, 27(12), 712\u2013719. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/j.tins.2004.10.007","journal-title":"Trends in Neurosciences"},{"key":"2763_CR26","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","unstructured":"Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1\u201326. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.18637\/jss.v082.i13 .","DOI":"10.18637\/jss.v082.i13"},{"issue":"6","key":"2763_CR27","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"1","DOI":"10.1371\/journal.pone.0130834","volume":"10","author":"K Lange","year":"2015","unstructured":"Lange, K., K\u00fchn, S., & Filevich, E. (2015). \u2018Just another tool for online studies\u2019 (JATOS): An easy solution for setup and management of web servers supporting online studies. PLOS ONE, 10(6), 1\u201314. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1371\/journal.pone.0130834","journal-title":"PLOS ONE"},{"key":"2763_CR28","unstructured":"Lenth, R. V. (2021). emmeans: Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means (Version 1.8.6) [Computer software]. https:\/\/cran.r-project.org\/package=emmeans"},{"issue":"4","key":"2763_CR29","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"492","DOI":"10.1037\/0033-295X.95.4.492","volume":"95","author":"GD Logan","year":"1988","unstructured":"Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automation. Psychological Review, 95(4), 492\u2013527.","journal-title":"Psychological Review"},{"issue":"1","key":"2763_CR30","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"20","DOI":"10.1016\/j.cogpsych.2007.11.001","volume":"57","author":"SA Los","year":"2008","unstructured":"Los, S. A., & Schut, M. L. J. (2008). The effective time course of preparation. Cognitive Psychology, 57(1), 20\u201355. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/j.cogpsych.2007.11.001","journal-title":"Cognitive Psychology"},{"key":"2763_CR31","unstructured":"Morey, R. D., & Rouder, J. N. (2018). BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes factors for common designs (Version 0.9.12.4.4) [Computer software]. https:\/\/cran.r-project.org\/package=BayesFactor"},{"issue":"5","key":"2763_CR32","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"437","DOI":"10.1111\/psyp.12190","volume":"51","author":"D Mullens","year":"2014","unstructured":"Mullens, D., Woodley, J., Whitson, L., Provost, A., Heathcote, A., Winkler, I., & Todd, J. (2014). Altering the primacy bias-How does a prior task affect mismatch negativity? Psychophysiology, 51(5), 437\u2013445. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1111\/psyp.12190","journal-title":"Psychophysiology"},{"issue":"6","key":"2763_CR33","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"668","DOI":"10.1111\/j.1469-8986.2005.00355.x","volume":"42","author":"D M\u00fcller","year":"2005","unstructured":"M\u00fcller, D., Widmann, A., & Schr\u00f6ger, E. (2005). Auditory streaming affects the processing of successive deviant and standard sounds. Psychophysiology, 42(6), 668\u2013676. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1111\/j.1469-8986.2005.00355.x","journal-title":"Psychophysiology"},{"issue":"1","key":"2763_CR34","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"1","DOI":"10.1016\/j.biopsycho.2004.03.018","volume":"68","author":"D M\u00fcller","year":"2005","unstructured":"M\u00fcller, D., Widmann, A., & Schr\u00f6ger, E. (2005). Deviance-repetition effects as a function of stimulus feature, feature value variation, and timing: A mismatch negativity study. Biological Psychology, 68(1), 1\u201314. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/j.biopsycho.2004.03.018","journal-title":"Biological Psychology"},{"issue":"2","key":"2763_CR35","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"109","DOI":"10.1016\/j.ijpsycho.2013.03.015","volume":"88","author":"P Paavilainen","year":"2013","unstructured":"Paavilainen, P. (2013). The mismatch-negativity (MMN) component of the auditory event-related potential to violations of abstract regularities: A review. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 88(2), 109\u2013123. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/j.ijpsycho.2013.03.015","journal-title":"International Journal of Psychophysiology"},{"issue":"5","key":"2763_CR36","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"715","DOI":"10.1097\/00001756-200304150-00011","volume":"14","author":"P Paavilainen","year":"2003","unstructured":"Paavilainen, P., Degerman, A., Takegata, R., & Winkler, I. (2003). Spectral and temporal stimulus characteristics in the processing of abstract auditory features. NeuroReport, 14(5), 715\u2013718. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1097\/00001756-200304150-00011","journal-title":"NeuroReport"},{"issue":"1","key":"2763_CR37","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"68","DOI":"10.1027\/1618-3169\/a000009","volume":"57","author":"FBR Parmentier","year":"2010","unstructured":"Parmentier, F. B. R., & Andr\u00e9s, P. (2010). The involuntary capture of attention by sound: Novelty and postnovelty distraction in young and older adults. Experimental Psychology, 57(1), 68\u201376. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1027\/1618-3169\/a000009","journal-title":"Experimental Psychology"},{"issue":"3","key":"2763_CR38","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"374","DOI":"10.1016\/j.cognition.2011.02.001","volume":"119","author":"FBR Parmentier","year":"2011","unstructured":"Parmentier, F. B. R., Elsley, J. V., Andr\u00e9s, P., & Barcel\u00f3, F. (2011). Why are auditory novels distracting? Contrasting the roles of novelty, violation of expectation and stimulus change. Cognition, 119(3), 374\u2013380. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/j.cognition.2011.02.001","journal-title":"Cognition"},{"issue":"2","key":"2763_CR39","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"347","DOI":"10.1016\/S0926-6410(03)00136-8","volume":"17","author":"U Roeber","year":"2003","unstructured":"Roeber, U., Widmann, A., & Schr\u00f6ger, E. (2003). Auditory distraction by duration and location deviants: A behavioral and event-related potential study. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(2), 347\u2013357. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/S0926-6410(03)00136-8","journal-title":"Cognitive Brain Research"},{"issue":"5","key":"2763_CR40","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"1519","DOI":"10.1007\/s00221-018-5237-z","volume":"236","author":"T Rosburg","year":"2018","unstructured":"Rosburg, T., Weigl, M., Thiel, R., & Mager, R. (2018). The event-related potential component P3a is diminished by identical deviance repetition, but not by non-identical repetitions. Experimental Brain Research, 236(5), 1519\u20131530. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1007\/s00221-018-5237-z","journal-title":"Experimental Brain Research"},{"issue":"2","key":"2763_CR41","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"225","DOI":"10.3758\/PBR.16.2.225","volume":"16","author":"JN Rouder","year":"2009","unstructured":"Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D., & Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(2), 225\u2013237. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.3758\/PBR.16.2.225","journal-title":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review"},{"issue":"5","key":"2763_CR42","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"356","DOI":"10.1016\/j.jmp.2012.08.001","volume":"56","author":"JN Rouder","year":"2012","unstructured":"Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., Speckman, P. L., & Province, J. M. (2012). Default Bayes factors for ANOVA designs. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 56(5), 356\u2013374. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/j.jmp.2012.08.001","journal-title":"Journal of Mathematical Psychology"},{"issue":"12","key":"2763_CR43","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"1149","DOI":"10.1097\/00001756-199212000-00030","volume":"3","author":"J Saarinen","year":"1992","unstructured":"Saarinen, J., Paavilainen, P., Schr\u00f6ger, E., Tervaniemi, M., & N\u00e4\u00e4t\u00e4nen, R. (1992). Representation of abstract attributes of auditory stimuli in the human brain. NeuroReport, 3(12), 1149\u20131151. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1097\/00001756-199212000-00030","journal-title":"NeuroReport"},{"issue":"4","key":"2763_CR44","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"434","DOI":"10.1111\/j.1469-8986.1984.tb00223.x","volume":"21","author":"M Sams","year":"1984","unstructured":"Sams, M., Alho, K., & N\u00e4\u00e4t\u00e4nen, R. (1984). Short-term habituation and dishabituation of the mismatch negativity of the ERP. Psychophysiology, 21(4), 434\u2013441. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1111\/j.1469-8986.1984.tb00223.x","journal-title":"Psychophysiology"},{"issue":"4","key":"2763_CR45","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"565","DOI":"10.1007\/s10548-013-0334-6","volume":"27","author":"E Schr\u00f6ger","year":"2014","unstructured":"Schr\u00f6ger, E., Bendixen, A., Denham, S. L., Mill, R. W., Bohm, T. M., & Winkler, I. (2014). Predictive regularity representations in violation detection and auditory stream segregation: From conceptual to computational models. Brain Topography, 27(4), 565\u2013577. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1007\/s10548-013-0334-6","journal-title":"Brain Topography"},{"key":"2763_CR46","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","unstructured":"Schr\u00f6ger, E., Bendixen, A., Trujillo-Barreto, N., & Roeber, U. (2007). Processing of abstract rule violations in audition. PLOS ONE, 2(11): e1131. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1371\/journal.pone.0001131","DOI":"10.1371\/journal.pone.0001131"},{"issue":"2","key":"2763_CR47","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"200","DOI":"10.1016\/j.ijpsycho.2011.11.003","volume":"83","author":"M Schwartze","year":"2012","unstructured":"Schwartze, M., Tavano, A., Schr\u00f6ger, E., & Kotz, S. A. (2012). Temporal aspects of prediction in audition: Cortical and subcortical neural mechanisms. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83(2), 200\u2013207. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/j.ijpsycho.2011.11.003","journal-title":"International Journal of Psychophysiology"},{"issue":"4","key":"2763_CR48","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"216","DOI":"10.1080\/00031305.1980.10483031","volume":"34","author":"SR Searle","year":"1980","unstructured":"Searle, S. R., Speed, F. M., & Milliken, G. A. (1980). Population Marginal Means in the Linear Model: An Alternative to Least Squares Means. The American Statistician, 34(4), 216\u2013221. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1080\/00031305.1980.10483031","journal-title":"The American Statistician"},{"key":"2763_CR49","unstructured":"Singmann, H., Bolker, B., Westfall, J., Aust, F., & Ben-Shachar, M. S. (2021). afex: Analysis of factorial experiments (Version 1.3.0) [Computer software]. https:\/\/cran.r-project.org\/package=afex"},{"issue":"3","key":"2763_CR50","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"431","DOI":"10.1016\/S0926-6410(01)00067-2","volume":"12","author":"E Sussman","year":"2001","unstructured":"Sussman, E., & Winkler, I. (2001). Dynamic sensory updating in the auditory system. Cognitive Brain Research, 12(3), 431\u2013439. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/S0926-6410(01)00067-2","journal-title":"Cognitive Brain Research"},{"issue":"2","key":"2763_CR51","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"308","DOI":"10.1111\/ejn.12404","volume":"39","author":"A Tavano","year":"2014","unstructured":"Tavano, A., Widmann, A., Bendixen, A., Trujillo-Barreto, N., & Schr\u00f6ger, E. (2014). Temporal regularity facilitates higher-order sensory predictions in fast auditory sequences. European Journal of Neuroscience, 39(2), 308\u2013318. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1111\/ejn.12404","journal-title":"European Journal of Neuroscience"},{"issue":"10","key":"2763_CR52","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"1434","DOI":"10.1111\/j.1469-8986.2011.01208.x","volume":"48","author":"J Todd","year":"2011","unstructured":"Todd, J., & Mullens, D. (2011). Implementing conditional inference in the auditory system: What matters? Psychophysiology, 48(10), 1434\u20131443. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1111\/j.1469-8986.2011.01208.x","journal-title":"Psychophysiology"},{"issue":"10","key":"2763_CR53","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"3009","DOI":"10.1016\/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.009","volume":"48","author":"J Todd","year":"2010","unstructured":"Todd, J., & Robinson, J. (2010). The use of conditional inference to reduce prediction error\u2014A mismatch negativity (MMN) study. Neuropsychologia, 48(10), 3009\u20133018. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.009","journal-title":"Neuropsychologia"},{"key":"2763_CR54","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"113","DOI":"10.1016\/j.brainres.2009.11.019","volume":"1310","author":"J Todd","year":"2010","unstructured":"Todd, J., Myers, R., Pirillo, R., & Drysdale, K. (2010). Neuropsychological correlates of auditory perceptual inference: A mismatch negativity (MMN) study. Brain Research, 1310, 113\u2013123. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/j.brainres.2009.11.019","journal-title":"Brain Research"},{"issue":"12","key":"2763_CR55","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"3399","DOI":"10.1016\/j.neuropsychologia.2011.08.016","volume":"49","author":"J Todd","year":"2011","unstructured":"Todd, J., Provost, A., & Cooper, G. (2011). Lasting first impressions: A conservative bias in automatic filters of the acoustic environment. Neuropsychologia, 49(12), 3399\u20133405. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/j.neuropsychologia.2011.08.016","journal-title":"Neuropsychologia"},{"key":"2763_CR56","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"1","DOI":"10.3389\/fnins.2014.00180","volume":"8","author":"J Todd","year":"2014","unstructured":"Todd, J., Heathcote, A., Whitson, L. R., Mullens, D., Provost, A., & Winkler, I. (2014). Mismatch negativity (MMN) to pitch change is susceptible to order-dependent bias. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 1\u20139. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.3389\/fnins.2014.00180","journal-title":"Frontiers in Neuroscience"},{"issue":"4","key":"2763_CR57","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"1","DOI":"10.1111\/psyp.13528","volume":"57","author":"J Todd","year":"2020","unstructured":"Todd, J., Frost, J., Fitzgerald, K., & Winkler, I. (2020). Setting precedent: Initial feature variability affects the subsequent precision of regularly varying sound contexts. Psychophysiology, 57(4), 1\u201314. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1111\/psyp.13528","journal-title":"Psychophysiology"},{"issue":"46","key":"2763_CR58","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"10440","DOI":"10.1523\/JNEUROSCI.1905-04.2004","volume":"24","author":"N Ulanovsky","year":"2004","unstructured":"Ulanovsky, N., Las, L., Farkas, D., & Nelken, I. (2004). Multiple time scales of adaptation in auditory cortex neurons. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(46), 10440\u201310453. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1523\/JNEUROSCI.1905-04.2004","journal-title":"Journal of Neuroscience"},{"issue":"8","key":"2763_CR59","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"1292","DOI":"10.1162\/jocn.2006.18.8.1292","volume":"18","author":"TL van Zuijen","year":"2006","unstructured":"van Zuijen, T. L., Simoens, V. L., Paavilainen, P., N\u00e4\u00e4t\u00e4nen, R., & Tervaniemi, M. (2006). Implicit, intuitive, and explicit knowledge of abstract regularities in a sound sequence: An event-related brain potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(8), 1292\u20131303. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1162\/jocn.2006.18.8.1292","journal-title":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience"},{"issue":"51","key":"2763_CR60","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"20754","DOI":"10.1073\/pnas.1117807108","volume":"108","author":"C Wacongne","year":"2011","unstructured":"Wacongne, C., Labyt, E., Van Wassenhove, V., Bekinschtein, T. A., Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2011). Evidence for a hierarchy of predictions and prediction errors in human cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(51), 20754\u201320759. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1073\/pnas.1117807108","journal-title":"Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America"},{"key":"2763_CR61","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","DOI":"10.1007\/978-3-319-24277-4","volume-title":"ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis","author":"H Wickham","year":"2016","unstructured":"Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis (2nd ed.). Switzerland: Springer. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1007\/978-3-319-24277-4","edition":"2"},{"key":"2763_CR62","unstructured":"Wickham, H., & Henry, L. (2020). tidyr: Tidy messy data (Version 1.3.0) [Computer software]. https:\/\/cran.r-project.org\/package=tidyr"},{"key":"2763_CR63","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","unstructured":"Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., McGowan, L. D., Fran\u00e7ois, R., Grolemund, G., Hayes, A., Henry, L., Hester, J., Kuhn, M., Pedersen, T. L., Miller, E., Bache, S. M., M\u00fcller, K., Ooms, J., Robinson, D., Seidel, D. P., Spinu, V., \u2026, Yutani, H. (2019). Welcome to the tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software, 4(43), 1686. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.21105\/joss.01686","DOI":"10.21105\/joss.01686"},{"key":"2763_CR64","doi-asserted-by":"crossref","unstructured":"Wickham, H., Fran\u00e7ois, R., Henry, L., & M\u00fcller, K. (2021). dplyr: A grammar of data manipulation (Version 1.1.2) [Computer software]. https:\/\/cran.r-project.org\/package=dplyr","DOI":"10.1007\/978-1-4842-6876-6_1"},{"issue":"3\/4","key":"2763_CR65","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"147","DOI":"10.1027\/0269-8803.21.34.147","volume":"21","author":"I Winkler","year":"2007","unstructured":"Winkler, I. (2007). Interpreting the mismatch negativity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 21(3\/4), 147\u2013163. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1027\/0269-8803.21.34.147","journal-title":"Journal of Psychophysiology"},{"issue":"2","key":"2763_CR66","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"132","DOI":"10.1016\/j.ijpsycho.2011.10.001","volume":"83","author":"I Winkler","year":"2012","unstructured":"Winkler, I., & Czigler, I. (2012). Evidence from auditory and visual event-related potential (ERP) studies of deviance detection (MMN and vMMN) linking predictive coding theories and perceptual object representations. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83(2), 132\u2013143. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/j.ijpsycho.2011.10.001","journal-title":"International Journal of Psychophysiology"},{"key":"2763_CR67","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","first-page":"1","DOI":"10.1016\/j.bandl.2015.05.003","volume":"148","author":"I Winkler","year":"2015","unstructured":"Winkler, I., & Schr\u00f6ger, E. (2015). Auditory perceptual objects as generative models: Setting the stage for communication by sound. Brain and Language, 148, 1\u201322. https:\/\/doi.org\/10.1016\/j.bandl.2015.05.003","journal-title":"Brain and Language"}],"container-title":["Attention, Perception, &amp; Psychophysics"],"original-title":[],"language":"en","link":[{"URL":"https:\/\/link.springer.com\/content\/pdf\/10.3758\/s13414-023-02763-9.pdf","content-type":"application\/pdf","content-version":"vor","intended-application":"text-mining"},{"URL":"https:\/\/link.springer.com\/article\/10.3758\/s13414-023-02763-9\/fulltext.html","content-type":"text\/html","content-version":"vor","intended-application":"text-mining"},{"URL":"https:\/\/link.springer.com\/content\/pdf\/10.3758\/s13414-023-02763-9.pdf","content-type":"application\/pdf","content-version":"vor","intended-application":"similarity-checking"}],"deposited":{"date-parts":[[2023,10,25]],"date-time":"2023-10-25T14:04:31Z","timestamp":1698242671000},"score":1,"resource":{"primary":{"URL":"https:\/\/link.springer.com\/10.3758\/s13414-023-02763-9"}},"subtitle":[],"short-title":[],"issued":{"date-parts":[[2023,8,2]]},"references-count":67,"journal-issue":{"issue":"8","published-print":{"date-parts":[[2023,11]]}},"alternative-id":["2763"],"URL":"http:\/\/dx.doi.org\/10.3758\/s13414-023-02763-9","relation":{},"ISSN":["1943-3921","1943-393X"],"issn-type":[{"value":"1943-3921","type":"print"},{"value":"1943-393X","type":"electronic"}],"subject":["Linguistics and Language","Sensory Systems","Language and Linguistics","Experimental and Cognitive Psychology"],"published":{"date-parts":[[2023,8,2]]},"assertion":[{"value":"7 July 2023","order":1,"name":"accepted","label":"Accepted","group":{"name":"ArticleHistory","label":"Article History"}},{"value":"2 August 2023","order":2,"name":"first_online","label":"First Online","group":{"name":"ArticleHistory","label":"Article History"}},{"order":1,"name":"Ethics","group":{"name":"EthicsHeading","label":"Declaration"}},{"value":"The authors declare that there are no competing interests to disclose.","order":2,"name":"Ethics","group":{"name":"EthicsHeading","label":"Conflicts of interest"}},{"value":"We report how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions (if any), all experimental manipulations, and all measures in the study.","order":3,"name":"Ethics","group":{"name":"EthicsHeading","label":"Research transparency"}}]}}