uni-leipzig-open-access/json/S0140525X22000747

1 line
3.7 KiB
Text
Raw Normal View History

2024-01-25 13:46:53 +00:00
{"status":"ok","message-type":"work","message-version":"1.0.0","message":{"indexed":{"date-parts":[[2023,2,18]],"date-time":"2023-02-18T05:20:15Z","timestamp":1676697615681},"reference-count":8,"publisher":"Cambridge University Press (CUP)","license":[{"start":{"date-parts":[[2023,2,17]],"date-time":"2023-02-17T00:00:00Z","timestamp":1676592000000},"content-version":"unspecified","delay-in-days":47,"URL":"https:\/\/www.cambridge.org\/core\/terms"}],"content-domain":{"domain":["cambridge.org"],"crossmark-restriction":true},"short-container-title":["Behav Brain Sci"],"published-print":{"date-parts":[[2023]]},"abstract":"<jats:title>Abstract<\/jats:title>\n\t <jats:p>Before claiming major differences between the communication systems of humans and other species, it is necessary to (1) overcome methodological limitations in the comparative study of communicative intentions; (2) account for mechanisms other than epistemic vigilance that may also sustain complex forms of communication; and (3) better differentiate between motivational and cognitive factors potentially affecting the emergence of open-ended communication.<\/jats:p>","DOI":"10.1017\/s0140525x22000747","type":"journal-article","created":{"date-parts":[[2023,2,17]],"date-time":"2023-02-17T08:06:01Z","timestamp":1676621161000},"update-policy":"http:\/\/dx.doi.org\/10.1017\/policypage","source":"Crossref","is-referenced-by-count":0,"title":["Primates unleashed"],"prefix":"10.1017","volume":"46","author":[{"ORCID":"http:\/\/orcid.org\/0000-0003-3539-1067","authenticated-orcid":false,"given":"Federica","family":"Amici","sequence":"first","affiliation":[]},{"given":"Katja","family":"Liebal","sequence":"additional","affiliation":[]}],"member":"56","published-online":{"date-parts":[[2023,2,17]]},"reference":[{"key":"S0140525X22000747_ref1","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","DOI":"10.1007\/s10764-022-00316-9"},{"key":"S0140525X22000747_ref8","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","DOI":"10.1111\/brv.12289"},{"key":"S0140525X22000747_ref6","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","DOI":"10.1080\/03949370.2021.2015453"},{"key":"S0140525X22000747_ref3","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","DOI":"10.1016\/j.cub.2008.01.040"},{"key":"S0140525X22000747_ref7","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","DOI":"10.1038\/ncomms10986"},{"key":"S0140525X22000747_ref5","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","DOI":"10.1126\/science.aaf8110"},{"key":"S0140525X22000747_ref2","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","DOI":"10.1038\/441303a"},{"key":"S0140525X22000747_ref4","doi-asserted-by":"publisher","DOI":"10.1016\/j.cub.2011.11.053"}],"container-title":["Behavioral and Brain Sciences"],"original-title":[],"language":"en","link":[{"URL":"https:\/\/www.cambridge.org\/core\/services\/aop-cambridge-core\/content\/view\/S0140525X22000747","content-type":"unspecified","content-version":"vor","intended-application":"similarity-checking"}],"deposited":{"date-parts":[[2023,2,17]],"date-time":"2023-02-17T08:06:02Z","timestamp":1676621162000},"score":1,"resource":{"primary":{"URL":"https:\/\/www.cambridge.org\/core\/product\/identifier\/S0140525X22000747\/type\/journal_article"}},"subtitle":[],"short-title":[],"issued":{"date-parts":[[2023]]},"references-count":8,"alternative-id":["S0140525X22000747"],"URL":"http:\/\/dx.doi.org\/10.1017\/s0140525x22000747","relation":{},"ISSN":["0140-525X","1469-1825"],"issn-type":[{"value":"0140-525X","type":"print"},{"value":"1469-1825","type":"electronic"}],"subject":["Behavioral Neuroscience","Physiology","Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology"],"published":{"date-parts":[[2023]]},"assertion":[{"value":"Copyright \u00a9 The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press","name":"copyright","label":"Copyright","group":{"name":"copyright_and_licensing","label":"Copyright and Licensing"}}],"article-number":"e2"}}